Everyone encounters problems in which solutions are neither readily available
nor readily derivable from acquired knowledge. This type of problem solving, frequently
experienced in daily life, is referred to as practical problem solving. Such problems can be
experienced at the work place, or in school, the household, stores, movie theaters, or
really, anywhere. There is no consensus on how to define practical problems encountered
in life, but building on a distinction made by Neisser (1976), Sternberg and his colleagues
(Sternberg, 1985a, 1997a; Wagner and Sternberg, 1986) have classified problems as
academic or practical in nature. Academic problems tend to be (a) formulated by others,
(b) well-defined, (c) complete in the information they provide, (d) characterized by
having only one correct answer, (e) characterized by having only one method of obtaining
the correct answer, (f ) disembedded from ordinary experience, and (g) of little or no
intrinsic interest.
Practical problems, in contrast to academic problems, tend to be (a) unformulated
or in need of reformulation, (b) of personal interest, (c) lacking in information necessary
for solution, (d) related to everyday experience, (e) poorly defined, (f ) characterized by
multiple "correct" solutions, each with liabilities as well as assets, and (g) characterized
by multiple methods for picking a problem solution. Given the differences in the nature
of academic and practical problems, it is no surprise that people who are adept at
solving one kind of problem may well not be adept at solving problems of the other
kind.
The cognitive skills that individuals exhibit in finding solutions to practical
problems may be referred to as practical cognitive skills (Baltes, Dittman-Kohli, and
Dixon, 1984; Berg, in press; Berg and Sternberg, 1985; Rogoff, 1982; Sternberg,
1985a, 1997a; Wagner, in press). When combined, these skills can be referred to as
practical cognition, which is defined as cognitive skills that serve to find a more optimal
fit between the individual and the demands of the individual's environment, whether
by adapting to the environment, changing (or shaping) the environment, or selecting a
different environment (Sternberg, 1985a; Sternberg, 1997a). The concept of practical
cognition takes into account the distinction presented above between academic and
practical tasks. The skills emphasized in formal schooling have limited value if they
cannot be used to address practical, everyday problems.
1.1.1 Research on practical problem-solving skill
The research on practical cognition is becoming more and more central to mainstream
psychology (see Berg and Klaczynski, 1996, for a review). Initially, the examination of
practical cognition issued from a concern that the cognition of adults functioning largely
outside the academic environment from the moment they obtained their academic
degrees and virtually for the rest of their lives, was evaluated primarily by traditional
tests of cognition constructed to predict academic success.
Various aspects of the meaning of the concept of practical cognition are expressed
in a number of diverse constructs. Some researchers define everyday cognition as a
specific expression of conventional skills that permit adaptive behavior within a distinct
class of everyday-life situations (e.g., Willis and Schaie, 1986), whereas others stress
the unique nature of practical skills (e.g., Neisser, 1976; Wagner, 1987). Most
psychological studies of practical skills focus on solving problems that are ill-structured
in their goals and solutions and are frequently encountered in daily life (at home, work,
and in dealing with people) (e.g., Cornelius and Caspi, 1987; Denney, 1989).
A number of studies have addressed the relation between practical and academic
cognition. These studies have been carried out in a wide range of settings, using a
variety of tasks, and with diverse populations. We review some examples of research on
problem solving and reasoning. For other reviews see Ceci and Roazzi (1994), Rogoff
and Lave (1984), Scribner and Cole (1981), Sternberg and Wagner (1986, 1994),
Voss, Perkins, and Segal (1991), and Wagner (in press). Taken together, these studies
show that skill measured in one setting (e.g., school) does not necessarily transfer to
another setting (e.g., real-world task).
|