Unfortunately, recent efforts to define and measure social cognition have not led
to any substantial improvement in our understanding of the construct. There appear to
be as many definitions and operationalizations of social cognition as there are researchers.
The definitions of social cognition reference dimensions such as social perception, social
knowledge, social insight, empathy, social memory, and social adaptation. Furthermore,
there is little consistency regarding the relationships among measures of social cognition
or their relations to measures of academic cognition. Although we acknowledge Cantor
and Harlow's (1994) concern regarding the difficulty in measuring social cognition,
the construct of tacit knowledge, elaborated upon in other chapters, represents an attempt
to quantify context-specific knowledge that is an aspect of practical cognition. Tests of
tacit knowledge have been successful in predicting performance (behavioral outcomes)
and discriminating practical from abstract or academic cognition (e.g., Sternberg et al.,
1993; Sternberg et al., 1995). Before considering the measurement of practical cognition,
we discuss another related construct, that of emotional cognition.
2.2 Emotional cognition
Research and theorizing on the construct of emotional cognition (also called, perhaps
inappropriately, "emotional intelligence") has a much shorter history in comparison to
social cognition. According to Mayer, Salovey, and Caruso (in press), the history of
emotional cognition research spans less than a decade. As such, the number of definitions
of and approaches to studying emotional cognition are delineated more readily.
Mayer et al. (in press) distinguished between two general models of emotional
cognition. Skill models view emotional cognition as the intersection of cognition and
emotion. Mixed models define emotional cognition as a combination of mental skill and
personality traits. We talk first about the mixed models (e.g., Bar-On, 1997; Goleman,
1995) and their associated measures of emotional cognition. Then we discuss the work
of Mayer and Salovey as they attempt to characterize emotional cognition as distinct
from personality.
Goleman (1995) brought popular attention to the concept of emotional cognition.
He argued, as other researchers have (e.g., Gardner, 1983; Sternberg, 1997a), that
cognitive-ability tests and similar tests (e.g., SATs) fail to predict accurately who will
succeed in life. Goleman suggested that part of the 80% variance in success unaccounted
for by overall cognitive ability could be explained by other characteristics, one of which
is emotional cognition. He defined emotional cognition as including "skills such as
being able to motivate oneself and persist in the face of frustrations; to control impulses
and delay gratification; to regulate one's moods and keep distress from swamping the
skill to think; to empathize and to hope" (p. 34). Although Goleman did not point to
any specific test of emotional cognition, he cited support for the construct in research
on related factors, such as empathy and ego resilience, which suggests that emotional
cognition is distinct from overall cognitive ability.
|