Grigorenko and Sternberg measured analytical skill using the Series and
the Matrices subtests of the Test of g: Culture Fair, Level II (Cattell,
1940; Cattell and Cattell, 1973) to measure fluid skills. A test of crystallized
cognition was adapted from existing
traditional tests of analogies and synonyms/antonyms used in Russia. Creative
cognition was measured by asking participants to describe the world through
the eyes of insects
and to describe who might live and what might happen on a fictitious planet called
"Priumliava." Responses were rated for novelty, quality, and sophistication.
For practical cognition, participants were asked to report their practical skills
in the social domain
(e.g., effective and successful communication with other people), in the family
domain (e.g., how to fix household items, how to run the family budget), and
in the domain of
effective resolution of sudden problems (e.g., organizing something that has
become chaotic). The participants were also asked to respond to 4 vignettes,
based on themes of
(1) how to maintain the value of one's savings; (2) what to do when one makes
a purchase and discovers that the item one has purchased is broken; (3) how to
locate medical
assistance in a time of need; and (4) how to manage a salary bonus one has received
for outstanding work. Participants were asked to select the best option among
five presented
for each vignette. The most frequently chosen option was used as the keyed answer.
Finally, self-report measures of physical and mental health were used to assess
successful adaptation. Participants received a summary score on their physical
health based on
reports of chronic illness and other debilitation injuries or diseases. They
also completed the Beck Anxiety Scale (BAS, Beck, Epstein, Brown, and Steer,
1988) and the Beck
Depression Inventory (BDI, Beck et al., 1961), as well as five items that measured
their self-efficacy for adaptation.
Grigorenko and Sternberg found that practical cognition consistently predicted
self-reported adaptive functioning on all indicators, with higher practical cognition
associated with better physical and mental health. Analytical cognition was associated
with lower anxiety and higher self-efficacy on two items. Creative cognition was
marginally associated with poorer physical health, but lower anxiety. When the data
was analyzed separately by gender, creative skill was found to associate with lower anxiety
for women, but poorer physical health and lower self-efficacy for men. The results
suggest that both analytical and practical cognition have a positive effect on adaptive
functioning. There is inconclusive evidence regarding the role of creative cognition.
2.4.2 Measures of practical cognition
In addition to the STAT and self-report questions, Sternberg and his colleagues have
developed measures targeted specifically at practical cognition (see Sternberg et al.,
1993; Sternberg et al., 1995; Wagner, 1985). Practical cognition is viewed as relevant
to successful performance of everyday problems, whether the problems are of a social,
emotional, or task-related nature. Therefore, measures of practical cognition hold promise
for elucidating some of the unexplained portion of success that have not been accounted
for by traditional cognition tests. Sternberg and his colleagues have taken a knowledgebased
approach to measuring practical cognition. Tacit knowledge, as an aspect of practical
cognition, is experience-based knowledge relevant to solving practical problems. As
such, tacit knowledge can pertain to social or emotional information. Therefore, tacit
knowledge may provide a common approach to understanding various forms of
nonacademic cognition. In the sections that follow, we delineate further the construct
of tacit knowledge, describe methods of measuring tacit knowledge, and review a program
of research that provides growing support for the validity of tacit knowledge, and
subsequently, practical cognition.
|