Klaczynski, Laipple, and Jurden (1992) studied practical cognition among
adolescents in college-preparatory or vocational-training tracks. Depending on the chosen
developmental life-track, adolescents in the two groups differed in their interpretation
of practical problem situations. In particular, vocational students were concerned
primarily with goals involving the acquisition of adult status, such as marriage, steady
employment, and independence. College-preparatory students, on the other hand,
reported more achievement-oriented goals, such as doing well in school, gaining admission
to quality colleges, and scoring well on entrance exams.
Belief in the plasticity and fluidity of human developmental goals throughout
the life span is also reflected by the notion that there is no single outcome or endpoint
to cognitive development in general, or to the development of practical cognition in
particular (e.g., Rogoff, 1982). The implication of this line of reasoning is that the
individual and his or her context form a complex systemic unit; changes in the unit
shape the content, dynamics, and adaptability of the individual's cognitive functioning
in specific contexts. Thus, there is no "ideal" trajectory of cognitive development, and
there is no optimal instrument assessing cognitive functioning equally well at all periods
of the life span.
1.2.4 Practical problem-solving strategies
One of the main research trajectories in the field of practical cognition focuses on strategies
utilized in problem solving. Among the central characteristics of strategies discussed in
the research literature of the past 20 years (Belmont and Butterfield, 1969; Berg,
1989; Brown, 1975; Flavell, 1970; Naus and Ornstein, 1983; Pressley, Forest-Pressley,
Faust and Miller, 1985) are selectivity, goal-directedness, and intentionality. Many
developmental researchers have been especially interested in strategy selection as both
an individual and a developmental indicator of everyday problem-solving performance
(e.g., Frederiksen, 1986; Frederiksen, Jensen, and Beaton, 1972; Lazarus and Folkman,
1984).
Most of the early developmental work on everyday problem solving has been
carried out under the assumption that individuals' chosen strategies can be compared
irrespective of the developmental variation in the goals motivating these strategies (Band
and Weisz, 1988; Berg, 1989; Cornelius and Caspi, 1987; Folkman et al., 1987). The
major theoretical hypothesis dominating the field is that greater experience with everyday
problems leads to better problem solving (Baltes et al., 1984, Denney, 1982). This
claim assumes that a particular type of strategy—e.g., primary control reflected in
independent coping and problem-focused action—is a more effective way of dealing
with various problems than is some other strategy—e.g., secondary control reflected in
reliance on others and emotion-focused action (Denney, 1989; Folkman et al., 1987).
For example, self-action was the strategy most frequently mentioned across all ages in a
study of reported everyday problems (Berg, Strough, Calderone, Sansone, and Weir,
1998). Problem-focused action was most frequently mentioned for hypothetical problems
(Blanchard-Fields, Jahnke, and Camp, 1995). Developmental differences have been
encountered, suggesting that secondary control strategies, emotion-focused strategies,
and dependence on others increases across early childhood (Band and Weisz, 1988),
with further elevation in later adulthood (Brandtstaedter and Greve, 1994; Denney
and Palmer, 1981; Folkman et al., 1987; Heckhausen and Schultz, 1995). For instance,
researchers (Band and Weisz, 1988) found that older children were more likely to use
secondary control strategies, such as efforts to modify the subjective psychological state
of the self to better suit the present conditions of the problem, whereas younger children
were more likely to use primary control strategies, such as efforts to influence the problem
so that it meets the problem solver's expectations.
|