Second, knowledge acquired under low environmental support is more likely to
have practical value. When knowledge must be acquired on one's own, the probability
increases that some individuals will fail to acquire it. When some individuals fail to
acquire knowledge, those who succeed may gain a comparative advantage. This advantage
is expected to be lower when the knowledge is highly supported by the environment
(i.e., explicity and effectively taught) because more people would be expected to acquire
and use it. At the same time, knowledge acquired through one's own experiences should
have more personal relevance to the types of situations one encounters in everyday life.
Finally, we associate knowledge acquired through experience with knowledge that
is procedural in structure. Because procedural knowledge is more difficult to articulate
and more poorly conveyed relative to declarative knowledge, its acquisition is more
likely to be a function of experiential learning. By the same token, knowledge acquired
through experience is more likely to be related to action because originally it was obtained
in the context of performing a practical, everyday task.
Each of these features is viewed as a continuous, rather than discrete, dimension
of tacit knowledge. That is, knowledge is not categorized as either possessing or not
possessing these features, but rather it is a matter of degree. Some knowledge may be
more well-supported by the environment than other knowledge. Similarly, some
knowledge may have more practical value to the individual than other knowledge.
Knowledge that is closer to one end of the continuum is considered more representative
of tacit knowledge.
3.3 What tacit knowledge is not
We have identified above the features that help describe what type of knowledge we
consider tacit knowledge to be. It is helpful also to distinguish tacit knowledge
conceptually from other related concepts such as job knowledge, general cognition, and
performance.
3.3.1 Tacit knowledge is not synonymous with job knowledge
Schmidt and Hunter (1993) suggested that tacit knowledge is merely a type of job
knowledge. Tacit knowledge and job knowledge are viewed more appropriately as
overlapping concepts. First, some, but not all, tacit knowledge pertains to job-related
activities. Tacit knowledge can pertain to any personally-valued activity, including
academic and social activities; it is more than job knowledge. Second, some, but not all,
job knowledge is tacit. Job knowledge includes declarative and procedural knowledge,
with some of the latter characterized as tacit. Job knowledge may be explicit and readily
verbalized, as in the rules for operating a lathe or the steps used to compute simple
interest, or the knowledge may be tacit, as in knowing what package design will likely
sell a product.
Measures of tacit knowledge have the potential to explain individual differences
in performance that are not explained by traditional measures of job knowledge, which
tend to assess more declarative, explicit forms of knowledge (see e.g., Schmidt and
Hunter, 1998). Individual differences in the skill or inclination to acquire and use tacit
knowledge make it a potentially useful construct for understanding intelligent behavior
in real-world settings, as well as for predicting success in such settings.
3.3.2 Tacit knowledge is not a proxy for general cognition
The skill or propensity to acquire tacit knowledge is viewed as a dimension of practical
cognition that conventional skill tests do not adequately measure. Overall cognitive
ability tests and similar tests, which are intended to measure so-called general cognition
(g), are composed of problems that can be characterized as largely academic or abstract.
|